Download BD chaurasia Human Anatomy All Books pdf. 3 Radiographic Neuroanatomy: A Working Atlas: 018: Medicine & Health Science Books @. Science BooksNeurologyMedicalMed SchoolMedical Technology. Basic science of anterior cruciate ligament injury and repair A. Kiapour, PhD, Research Fellow 1 and M. Murray, MD, Associate Professor 1 1 Boston Children’s Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Sports Medicine Research Laboratory, Department of Orthopaedic Surgery, 300 Longwood Avenue, Boston, Massachusetts 02115, USA. You are here: Home → instructionalframework → officialframework → Health Careers Education → Anterior Anatomy and the Science of a natural Smile (SAA) Navigation Instructional Framework.
'Find out how to create natural looking anterior restorations.' If you are one of the 34,000 dental technicians in the USA, then you don't have a way to learn about anterior esthetics and how to create lifelike restorations. You have learned your craft by the 'watch me' over-the-shoulder technique of watching another technician. What you can learn is limited to the skill of the technician you are watching.
Training classes offered by manufacturers usually focus on how to use their product. The techniques taught usually apply to their products. Training classes taught by gurus do not include any additional books or guides, so you can easily forget exactly what the guru did. Does it frustrate you that your anterior restorations don't look as lifelike as you want them to? How would you like it if all the anterior restorations you create look lifelike and mimic the esthetic qualities of natural smiles? Imagine - not having to guess on how to make natural looking smiles. Your dentists and boss are happy with your anterior cases and do not wish that they looked better.
Anterior Anatomy and The Science of a Natural Smile is a revolutionary study of ten underlying esthetic components common to all anterior restorations. Created by John Ness, founder of PTC, this breakthrough material was extracted through the study of hundreds of natural smiles.
This monumental book will show you. ten essential principles to use to improve the esthetic quality of every anterior case. how these ten principles interact to create natural balance and harmony. the subtleties of anterior anatomy you need to know for esthetic contouring Developed for dental technicians, dental assistants, dental hygienists, dentists, and all dental professionals, this book presents the information you need to know about anterior dental anatomy and anterior esthetics. Successful dental labs and dental offices use The Science of a Natural Smile to establish a common vocabulary to effectively communicate about their cases.
With this vocabulary, misunderstandings and errors from miscommunication are reduced. Get the book that shows you the esthetic components you need to know to create lifelike anterior restorations.
A mirror neuron, or cubelli neuron, is a that both when an animal acts and when the animal observes the same action performed by another. Thus, the neuron 'mirrors' the behavior of the other, as though the observer were itself acting. Such neurons have been directly observed in species.
Birds have been shown to have imitative resonance behaviors and neurological evidence suggests the presence of some form of mirroring system. In humans, brain activity consistent with that of mirror neurons has been found in the, the, the and the.
The function of the mirror system in humans is a subject of much speculation. Some researchers in cognitive neuroscience and consider that this system provides the physiological mechanism for the perception/action coupling (see the ). They argue that mirror neurons may be important for understanding the actions of other people, and for learning new skills by imitation.
Some researchers speculate that mirror systems may simulate observed actions, and thus contribute to skills, while others relate mirror neurons to abilities. Neuroscientists such as Marco Iacoboni (UCLA) have argued that mirror neuron systems in the help us understand the actions and intentions of other people. In a study published in March 2005 Iacoboni and his colleagues reported that mirror neurons could discern whether another person who was picking up a cup of tea planned to drink from it or clear it from the table.
In addition, Iacoboni has argued that mirror neurons are the neural basis of the human capacity for emotions such as. However, there are scientists who express skepticism about the theories being advanced to explain the function of mirror neurons.
In a 2013 article for Wired, Christian Jarrett cautioned that.mirror neurons are an exciting, intriguing discovery – but when you see them mentioned in the media, remember that most of the research on these cells has been conducted in monkeys. Remember too that there are many different types of mirror neuron. And that we're still trying to establish for sure whether they exist in humans, and how they compare with the monkey versions. As for understanding the functional significance of these cells don't be fooled: that journey has only just begun. To date, no widely accepted neural or computational models have been put forward to describe how mirror neuron activity supports cognitive functions.
The subject of mirror neurons continues to generate intense debate. In 2014, published a special issue entirely devoted to mirror neuron research. Contents. Discovery In the 1980s and 1990s, neurophysiologists, Giuseppe Di Pellegrino, Leonardo Fogassi, and at the placed in the of the monkey to study specialized for the control of hand and mouth actions; for example, taking hold of an object and manipulating it. During each experiment, the researchers allowed the monkey to reach for pieces of food, and recorded from single neurons in the monkey's brain, thus measuring the neuron's response to certain movements. They found that some neurons responded when the monkey observed a person picking up a piece of food, and also when the monkey itself picked up the food. The discovery was initially sent to, but was rejected for its 'lack of general interest' before being published in a less competitive journal.
A few years later, the same group published another empirical paper, discussing the role of the mirror-neuron system in action recognition, and proposing that the human was the region of the monkey ventral premotor cortex. While these papers reported the presence of mirror neurons responding to hand actions, a subsequent study by Pier Francesco Ferrari and colleagues described the presence of mirror neurons responding to mouth actions and facial gestures. Further experiments confirmed that about 10% of neurons in the monkey inferior frontal and inferior parietal cortex have 'mirror' properties and give similar responses to performed hand actions and observed actions. In 2002 and colleagues reported that, in both humans and monkeys, the mirror system also responds to the sound of actions. Reports on mirror neurons have been widely published and confirmed with mirror neurons found in both inferior frontal and inferior parietal regions of the brain. Recently, evidence from strongly suggests that humans have similar mirror neurons systems: researchers have identified brain regions which respond during both action and observation of action.
Not surprisingly, these brain regions include those found in the macaque monkey However, (fMRI) can examine the entire brain at once and suggests that a much wider network of brain areas shows mirror properties in humans than previously thought. These additional areas include the and are thought to make the observer feel what it feels like to move in the observed way. Origin The most common theory behind the origin of mirror neuron is the genetic account which suggests that the mirrorness of mirror neurons is due primarily to heritable genetic factors and that the genetic predisposition to develop Mirror neuron evolved because they facilitate action understanding. The other theories as to the origin of mirror neurons include, and. In monkeys. Neonatal (newborn) macaque imitating facial expressions The first animal in which researchers have studied mirror neurons individually is the. In these monkeys, mirror neurons are found in the (region F5) and the.
Mirror neurons are believed to mediate the understanding of other animals'. For example, a mirror neuron which fires when the monkey rips a piece of paper would also fire when the monkey sees a person rip paper, or hears paper ripping (without visual cues). These properties have led researchers to believe that mirror neurons encode abstract concepts of actions like 'ripping paper', whether the action is performed by the monkey or another animal. The function of mirror neurons in macaques remains unknown. Adult macaques do not seem to learn by imitation.
Recent experiments by Ferrari and colleagues suggest that infant macaques can imitate a human's face movements, though only as and during a limited temporal window. Even if it has not yet been empirically demonstrated, it has been proposed that mirror neurons underlie this behaviour and other imitative phenomena. Indeed, there is limited understanding of the degree to which monkeys show imitative behaviour. In adult monkeys, mirror neurons may enable the monkey to understand what another monkey is doing, or to recognise the other monkey's action.
In humans. Diagram of the brain, showing the locations of the frontal and parietal lobes of the, viewed from the left.
The inferior frontal lobe is the lower part of the blue area, and the superior parietal lobe is the upper part of the yellow area. It is not normally possible to study single neurons in the human brain, so most evidence for mirror neurons in humans is indirect. Brain imaging experiments using (fMRI) have shown that the human and are active when the person performs an action and also when the person sees another individual performing an action. It has been suggested that these brain regions contain mirror neurons, and they have been defined as the human mirror neuron system. More recent experiments have shown that even at the level of single participants, scanned using fMRI, large areas containing multiple fMRI voxels increase their activity both during the observation and execution of actions. Neuropsychological studies looking at lesion areas that cause action knowledge, pantomime interpretation, and biological motion perception deficits have pointed to a causal link between the integrity of the inferior frontal gyrus and these behaviours. Studies have confirmed this as well.
These results indicate the activation in mirror neuron related areas are unlikely to be just epiphenomenal. A study published in April 2010 reports recordings from single neurons with mirror properties in the human brain. Mukamel et al. (Current Biology, 2010) recorded from the brains of 21 patients who were being treated at Ronald Reagan UCLA Medical Center for intractable epilepsy. The patients had been implanted with intracranial depth electrodes to identify seizure foci for potential surgical treatment. Electrode location was based solely on clinical criteria; the researchers, with the patients' consent, used the same electrodes to 'piggyback' their research. The researchers found a small number of neurons that fired or showed their greatest activity both when the individual performed a task and when they observed a task.
Other neurons had anti-mirror properties, that is, they responded when the participant performed an action but were inhibited when the participant saw that action. The mirror neurons found were located in the supplementary motor area and medial temporal cortex (other brain regions were not sampled). For purely practical reasons, these regions are not the same as those in which mirror neurons had been recorded from in the monkey: researchers in Parma were studying the ventral premotor cortex and the associated inferior parietal lobe, two regions in which epilepsy rarely occurs, and hence, single cell recordings in these regions are not usually done in humans. On the other hand, no one has to date looked for mirror neurons in the supplementary motor area or the medial temporal lobe in the monkey.
Together, this therefore does not suggest that humans and monkeys have mirror neurons in different locations, but rather that they may have mirror neurons both in the ventral premotor cortex and inferior parietal lobe, where they have been recorded in the monkey, and in the supplementary motor areas and medial temporal lobe, where they have been recorded from in human – especially because detailed human fMRI analyses suggest activity compatible with the presence of mirror neurons in all these regions. Another study has suggested that human beings don't necessarily have more mirror neurons than monkeys, but instead that there is a core set of mirror neurons used in action observation and execution. However, for other proposed functions of mirror neurons the mirror system may have the ability to recruit other areas of the brain when doing its auditory, somatosensory, and affective components. Doubts concerning mirror neurons Although many in the scientific community have expressed excitement about the discovery of mirror neurons, there are scientists who have expressed doubts about both the existence and role of mirror neurons in humans. According to scientists such as Hickok, Pascolo, and Dinstein, it is not clear whether mirror neurons really form a distinct class of cells (as opposed to an occasional phenomenon seen in cells that have other functions), and whether mirror activity is a distinct type of response or simply an artifact of an overall facilitation of the motor system. In 2008, Ilan Dinstein et al.
Argued that the original analyses were unconvincing because they were based on qualitative descriptions of individual cell properties, and did not take into account the small number of strongly mirror-selective neurons in motor areas. Other scientists have argued that the measurements of neuron fire delay seem not to be compatible with standard reaction times, and pointed out that nobody has reported that an interruption of the motor areas in F5 would produce a decrease in action recognition. (Critics of this argument have replied that these authors have missed human neuropsychological and studies reporting disruption of these areas do indeed cause action deficits without affecting other kinds of perception.) In 2009, Lingnau et al. Carried out an experiment in which they compared motor acts that were first observed and then executed to motor acts that were first executed and then observed. They concluded that there was a significant asymmetry between the two processes that indicated that mirror neurons do not exist in humans. They stated 'Crucially, we found no signs of adaptation for motor acts that were first executed and then observed. Failure to find cross-modal adaptation for executed and observed motor acts is not compatible with the core assumption of mirror neuron theory, which holds that action recognition and understanding are based on motor simulation.'
However, in the same year, Kilner et al. Showed that if goal directed actions are used as stimuli, both IPL and premotor regions show the repetition suppression between observation and execution that is predicted by mirror neurons. In 2009, Greg Hickok published an extensive argument against the claim that mirror neurons are involved in action-understanding: 'Eight Problems for the Mirror Neuron Theory of Action Understanding in Monkeys and Humans.' He concluded that 'The early hypothesis that these cells underlie action understanding is likewise an interesting and prima facie reasonable idea. However, despite its widespread acceptance, the proposal has never been adequately tested in monkeys, and in humans there is strong empirical evidence, in the form of physiological and neuropsychological (double-) dissociations, against the claim.' The mirror neurons can be activated only after the goal of the observed action has been attributed by other brain structures.
Vladimir Kosonogov sees another contradiction. The proponents of mirror neuron theory of action understanding postulate that the mirror neurons code the goals of others actions because they are activated if the observed action is goal-directed. However, the mirror neurons are activated only when the observed action is goal-directed (object-directed action or a communicative gesture, which certainly has a goal too). How do they 'know' that the definite action is goal-directed? At what stage of their activation do they detect a goal of the movement or its absence? In his opinion, the mirror neuron system can be activated only after the goal of the observed action is attributed by some other brain structures.
Neurophilosophers such as have expressed both scientific and philosophical objections to the theory that mirror neurons are responsible for understanding the intentions of others. In chapter 5 of her 2011 book, Braintrust, Churchland points out that the claim that mirror neurons are involved in understanding intentions (through simulating observed actions) is based on assumptions that are clouded by unresolved philosophical issues. She makes the argument that intentions are understood (coded) at a more complex level of neural activity than that of individual neurons.
Churchland states that 'A neuron, though computationally complex, is just a neuron. It is not an intelligent homunculus. If a neural network represents something complex, such as an intention to insult, it must have the right input and be in the right place in the neural circuitry to do that'. Recently, Cecilia Heyes (Professor of Experimental Psychology, Oxford) has advanced the theory that mirror neurons are the byproduct of associative learning as opposed to evolutionary adaptation.
She argues that mirror neurons in humans are the product of social interaction and not an evolutionary adaptation for action-understanding. In particular, Heyes rejects the theory advanced by V.S. Ramachandran that mirror neurons have been 'the driving force behind the great leap forward in human evolution.' Development Human infant data using eye-tracking measures suggest that the mirror neuron system develops before 12 months of age, and that this system may help human infants understand other people's actions. A critical question concerns how mirror neurons acquire mirror properties. Two closely related models postulate that mirror neurons are trained through or (see ).
However, if premotor neurons need to be trained by action in order to acquire mirror properties, it is unclear how newborn babies are able to mimic the facial gestures of another person (imitation of unseen actions), as suggested by the work of and Moore. One possibility is that the sight of tongue protrusion recruits an in neonates. Careful analysis suggests that 'imitation' of this single gesture may account for almost all reports of facial mimicry by new-born infants. Possible functions Understanding intentions Many studies link mirror neurons to understanding goals and intentions.
Fogassi et al. (2005) recorded the activity of 41 mirror neurons in the inferior parietal lobe (IPL) of two rhesus macaques. The IPL has long been recognized as an association cortex that integrates sensory information. The monkeys watched an experimenter either grasp an apple and bring it to his mouth or grasp an object and place it in a cup. In total, 15 mirror neurons fired vigorously when the monkey observed the 'grasp-to-eat' motion, but registered no activity while exposed to the 'grasp-to-place' condition.
For 4 other mirror neurons, the reverse held true: they activated in response to the experimenter eventually placing the apple in the cup but not to eating it. Only the type of action, and not the kinematic force with which models manipulated objects, determined neuron activity. It was also significant that neurons fired before the monkey observed the human model starting the second motor act (bringing the object to the mouth or placing it in a cup). Therefore, IPL neurons 'code the same act (grasping) in a different way according to the final goal of the action in which the act is embedded'. They may furnish a neural basis for predicting another individual's subsequent actions and inferring intention. Learning facilitation Another possible function of mirror neurons would be facilitation of learning. The mirror neurons code the concrete representation of the action, i.e., the representation that would be activated if the observer acted.
This would allow us to simulate (to repeat internally) the observed action implicitly (in the brain) to collect our own motor programs of observed actions and to get ready to reproduce the actions later. It is implicit training. Due to this, the observer will produce the action explicitly (in his/her behavior) with agility and finesse. This happens due to associative learning processes. The more frequently a synaptic connection is activated, the stronger it becomes. Empathy Stephanie Preston and, and and have independently argued that the mirror neuron system is involved in.
A large number of experiments using fMRI, (EEG) and (MEG) have shown that certain brain regions (in particular the anterior, and inferior frontal cortex) are active when people experience an emotion (disgust, happiness, pain, etc.) and when they see another person experiencing an emotion. And have also put forward the idea that this function of the mirror neuron system is crucial for experiences.
However, these brain regions are not quite the same as the ones which mirror hand actions, and mirror neurons for emotional states or empathy have not yet been described in monkeys. More recently, Christian Keysers at the and colleagues have shown that people who are more empathic according to self-report questionnaires have stronger activations both in the mirror system for hand actions and the mirror system for emotions, providing more direct support for the idea that the mirror system is linked to empathy. Some researchers observed that the human mirror system does not passively respond to the observation of actions but is influenced by the mindset of the observer.
Researchers observed the link of the mirror neurons during empathetic engagement in patient care. Human self awareness has speculated that mirror neurons may provide the neurological basis of human self-awareness. In an essay written for the Edge Foundation in 2009 Ramachandran gave the following explanation of his theory: '. I also speculated that these neurons can not only help simulate other people's behavior but can be turned 'inward'—as it were—to create second-order representations or meta-representations of your own earlier brain processes.
This could be the neural basis of introspection, and of the reciprocity of self awareness and other awareness. There is obviously a chicken-or-egg question here as to which evolved first, but. The main point is that the two co-evolved, mutually enriching each other to create the mature representation of self that characterizes modern humans'. Language In humans, functional MRI studies have reported finding areas homologous to the monkey mirror neuron system in the inferior frontal cortex, close to Broca's area, one of the hypothesized language regions of the brain. This has led to suggestions that human language evolved from a gesture performance/understanding system implemented in mirror neurons.
Mirror neurons have been said to have the potential to provide a mechanism for action-understanding, imitation-learning, and the simulation of other people's behaviour. This hypothesis is supported by some homologies between monkey premotor area F5 and human Broca's area. Rates of expansion link to the ability of to vocally mirror non-words and so to acquire the new word pronunciations.
Such occurs automatically, fast and separately in the brain to. Moreover, such vocal imitation can occur without comprehension such as in and. Further evidence for this link comes from a recent study in which the brain activity of two participants was measured using fMRI while they were gesturing words to each other using hand gestures with a game of – a modality that some have suggested might represent the evolutionary precursor of human language.